How the Brain Recognizes Faces
by Viatcheslav Wlassoff, PhD | October 17, 2015Face perception has played a central role for social interaction for millions of years, informing us about the identity, age, gender, mood, attractiveness, race and friendliness of a person. Scientists have suggested that facial recognition is a complex process, and it provides important access to the understanding of human brain function.
The brain identifies items within milliseconds and this fast recognition works because our brain is continually making predictions about objects in our field of view and then comparing these with incoming information. The details are far from clear, but a number of interesting recent findings provide a better insight on what’s going on in our brain when we see a face.
There are three essential phases in recognizing human faces. The first phase consists on coding of the physical characteristics of the person we are watching. The second phase is based on encoding the identity of the person, which give us a signal that this is a person we know. Then, in the final third phase, we recognize the person, but we do not know whether we know the name of this person,so we associate the face with the name of a person.
Researchers have also discovered that the certain parts of the brain are activated during those three phases of facial recognition. If there is damage to one of these areas, which can occur in some diseases or traumas, a person could become no longer able to recognize faces.
Dealing with visual perception and recognition of shapes, psychologists have noted a long ago that the perception and recognition of faces is special, cognitively-demanding “case” of perception.
In simple words, recognition of objects comes down to comparison of what we see with the appropriate mental picture, stored in long-term memory.
When we take a look at the object called “chair” we will recognize it as such because its characteristics correspond to our stored mental picture of the chair. Although not all chairs are the same and some copies of this object may differ from a mental prototype that serves as a standard for the comparison, each chair retains some of the key characteristics necessary for easy recognition. Basically, the object is correctly recognized the moment it is properly classified, i.e. when “stored” in the appropriate category.
Classification of perceptions with which the pattern recognition process generally ends in the case of faces often is just the beginning. Namely, in the encounter with a person it is not enough to recognize her or his face as a form that belongs to the category of “human face”. Almost immediately, we recognize her or his sex, possibly racial origin, age, and “decide” whether we like that person or not … Often, it is necessary to immediately realize whether the face is known to us or not. If so, we draw from memory a series of semantic information about it: from where do we know it, what is it called, what it does.
Fusiform gyrus
Scientists discovered that a brain tissue which becomes activated when people look at a face is the fusiform gyrus. In a brain scan, this area “lights up”, or becomes active, more powerfully than it does when participants look at other objects.
Studies shows the connection between the fusiform gyrus and its role in the recognition of faces after several dozen patients with damage of this tissue lost their ability to identify once known faces. Scientists believe that fusiform gyrus helps us to recognize faces as a category or that it could help the person distinguish individual faces. According to the new studies, autistic people did not have more activity in this region for faces comparing to other objects, because people with autism generally do not look at faces, so there are suggestions that this area can get strengthened with practice.
Mooney face studies
Mooney faces are photographs named by their inventor Craig Mooney. These are photographs of faces which have been reduced completely to black and white. One Mooney face study showed that facial recognition performance in participants became noticeably poorer and slower, firstly because in those photographs, participants don’t see faces oriented upright, and secondly, light doesn’t comes from above.
The “Predictive Coding” theory, suggests that “in this situation neural signals only have to be sent to higher areas of brain for processing if predictions aren’t met. So an increase in neural signal activity near higher brain areas should occur. However, there are also competing theories which predict the exact opposite.”
Visual pareidolia
Pareidolia is a type of illusion or misperception involving a vague or obscure stimulus being perceived as something clear and distinct. Visual pareidolia is an excellent example of pareidolia, which we certainly all experience, and it is based on recognition of different animals or persons (mostly their faces), for example in the clouds.
Pareidolia is an integral part of human nature and everyday life, and it is not considered as a disorder. It is believed that it is the product of human evolution, because the recognition of a human faces was one of the most important skills for survival and development of a mankind. This is why we most common “recognize” human faces in random stimuli. There are well known examples of religious pareidolia where people from Christian culture “recognize” the faces of Jesus, the Virgin Mary or saints in the wood, stains on the wall etc. There are also cases of sightings of ghosts on the photos or images, and numerous cases of UFO sightings also attributed to this phenomenon.
Scientist believed that pareidolia is a result of the way that our brain processes information. The brain constantly filters lines, shapes and colors, giving them meaning, making meaningful images and linking them with the acquired knowledge. However, sometimes, we associate ambiguous items with something that can easily be named and identified.
References
Brodski, A., Paasch, G., Helbling, S., & Wibral, M. (2015). The Faces of Predictive Coding Journal of Neuroscience, 35 (24), 8997-9006 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1529-14.2015
Gobbini MI, & Haxby JV (2007). Neural systems for recognition of familiar faces. Neuropsychologia, 45 (1), 32-41 PMID: 16797608
Haxby, J., Ungerleider, L., Horwitz, B., Maisog, J., Rapoport, S., & Grady, C. (1996). Face encoding and recognition in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93 (2), 922-927 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.93.2.922
Lewis, M., & Ellis, H. (2003). How we detect a face: A survey of psychological evidence International Journal of Imaging Systems and Technology, 13 (1), 3-7 DOI: 10.1002/ima.10040
Liu, J., Harris, A., & Kanwisher, N. (2010). Perception of Face Parts and Face Configurations: An fMRI Study Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22 (1), 203-211 DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21203
Image via Corepics VOF / Shutterstock.
Share This Article
New ‘Brain Games’ Exclusive Preview – Times Square Mass Mind Reading
This Sunday February 14th (9 p.m. ET), the Emmy-nominated Brain Games tv-show is back! Wonder junkie Jason Silva returns to our screens, teaming up with... READ MORE →
Like what you read? Give to Brain Blogger sponsored by GNIF with a tax-deductible donation.
Make A Donation